TED Talks Wonât Treat Your Depression
Qatar Tribune
Jesse Singal NYT Maybe chicken soup can treat depression. That provocative claim was made not by a quack in a late-night infomercial but by John Bargh, a ...
Jesse SingalNYTMaybe chicken soup can treat depression. That provocative claim was made not by a quack in a late-night infomercial but by John Bargh, a Yale social psychologist. He is an expert on âsocial primes,â the subtle cues that supposedly exert a major unconscious influence on our behavior. He has published research suggesting that, for example, exposure to words with geriatric associations like âwrinklesâ primes people to walk slower.In his 2017 book, âBefore You Know It: The Unconscious Reasons We Do What We Do,â Dr. Bargh mentions a clinical trial in which severely depressed patients in a mental health facility appeared to improve after exposure to very high temperatures. He speculates that perhaps an outpatient approach involving soup could do the trick, too, since âthe warmth of the soup helps replace the social warmth that may be missing from the personâs life.â Such remedies âare unlikely to make big profits for the pharmaceutical and psychiatric industries,â he writes, but they warrant further research.Psychologists are welcome to research whatever they like. But if you have loved someone afflicted with treatment-resistant depression, as I have, this seems far-fetched: Every day, millions of depressed people drink coffee to no apparent salutary effect.Dr Barghâs idea is one of the purest distillations of what I call âprimeworld,â a myopic but seductive worldview. It suggests that human behavior is shaped rather easily by primes and other subtle influences â an irrational bias here, a too-pessimistic mind-set there â and that these influences can often be easily dispelled with low-cost psychological tweaks that target individuals to help solve societal problems. This understanding of society has flourished as a result of a general fascination with easily digestible pop-psychology nuggets â often delivered via TED Talks and best-selling books â that appears to have exploded in recent years.But because it ignores the bigger, more structural forces that do far more to influence human behavior (from our exposure to early-life trauma to how much money we have to whether we grew up in a segregated neighborhood), it is flawed. Worse, it might actually be hindering our ability to solve real-world problems. And now, with a replication crisis felling once highly regarded psychological findings, itâs probably time to be a little bit more skeptical of these quick-fix ideas.It isnât just psychologists who profit from this approach: Thereâs an influential ecosystem of journalists, pundits and other professional âthought leadersâ who benefit from page views, book deals or the exposure offered by a viral TED Talk. And these solutions, often framed as inexpensive and politically uncontroversial, are catnip not only to everyday consumers of pop science but also to policymakers hungry for quick fixes.To be sure, there are kernels of truth to some of these ideas. At the margins, slight changes to our environment or a presentation of choices can affect our behavior. Some successful so-called nudges make people a bit more likely to save energy or a bit less likely to grab an extra doughnut in line at a college cafeteria.But some psychologists have made much bolder claims â which are much less credible. For Dr. Bargh, for example, seeing cleaner streets spurs prosocial behavior in a manner that, he says, can help explain New York Cityâs great violent-crime drop that started in the 1990s. (Thatâs why he lauds âbroken windowsâ policing.) He acknowledges there were other factors, but he also states flatly in his book that the cityâs resurgence âwas a result of a new culture of cues for positive behavior being instituted.âThis is a vast oversimplification of a complicated problem. Few criminologists believe that these sorts of cues for positive behavior can tell us much, if anything, about the great crime decline.Other recent blockbuster ideas in psychology are also steeped in this ideology. Take mind-set interventions, which are designed to shift peopleâs mind-sets from âfixedâ (âI failed the test â Iâm just stupidâ) to âgrowthâ (âIâll do better next time if I work harderâ). âFor 30 years, my research has shown that the view you adopt for yourself profoundly affects the way you lead your life,â said Carol Dweck, the originator of that idea and a professor at Stanford, in her book âMindset: The New Psychology of Successâ (ideas she echoed in a TED Talk that has been viewed more than 12.5 million times).Perhaps better known is the implicit association test, which claims to be able to measure oneâs level of implicit (or unconscious) bias via a brief computer test that involves comparing reaction time to different stimuli. Since the creators of the test also claimed that implicit bias can go a long way toward explaining persistent racially discriminatory gaps in the United States, the computer test is now one of the most common features of contemporary diversity trainings. Since these claims were first made, though, a full-blown replication crisis has hit psychology, meaning that when researchers attempt to redo previous studies, they often find either a much less impressive result or none whatsoever. It turns out that the standard statistical methods long employed by psychologists (and other scientists) can easily produce false positive results. About 50 percent of published results from experimental psychology have failed to replicate, and the subfield of social psychology â the home base of most social priming, implicit bias and stereotype-threat research â tends to fare even worse.Studies purporting to offer simple remedies to serious problems have been hit particularly hard. Mind-set interventions appear to be nowhere near as powerful as Professor Dweck initially advertised: A major, well-constructed 2019 study in Nature found some effect, but only a relatively small one and only for weaker students. The implicit association test has experienced similar travails. It is still often a part of everyday diversity-training settings, but its creators long ago acknowledged that it is too noisy a test to be used to identify people as likely to engage in racist acts (which constitutes significant backtracking from their original claim).Sometimes people mistakenly believe that the best or truest scientific ideas rise to the top of the heap â that popularity implies accuracy and rigor. This has never quite been the case, but itâs an even more questionable claim in an era in which pop science is so hotly marketable via TED Talks and other platforms. Often the ideas that reach those heights are the ones that we most want to believe. And weâd like to think that we can fix the world easily.More Related News